Home » 8400 week 6 A

8400 week 6 A

4/5

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/wicked-deeds/201403/the-twisted-tale-the-co-ed-killer

CASE STUDY

The Flawed Interview of a Psychopathic Killer:
What Went Wrong?

FRANK S. PERRI*
Criminal Legal Department, Rockford, IL, USA

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Abstract

The fl awed interview of convicted killer Christopher Porco illustrates what occurs when
a suspect’s psychopathic traits are not considered in formulating an interview strategy.
Specifi c areas of the dialogue between Christopher Porco and law enforcement demon-
strates that traditional methods of interviewing may not produce a confession or valuable
information when it comes to psychopathic suspects. Although the traditional goal of an
interview is to obtain a confession or valuable information, law enforcement will have to
display fl exibility in the interview process by implementing alternative interview strategies
and redefi ning what a successful interview is composed of when these suspects are unwill-
ing to confess to their crimes. Failure to remain fl exible, as displayed in the Christopher
Porco case, which yielded neither a confession nor valuable information, can jeopardise
the strength of a homicide case that ultimately may go to trial. Copyright © 2010 John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Key words: interview; interrogation; psychopathy; homicide; Reid technique

INTRODUCTION

From a law enforcement perspective, the psychopath can be described as one of their
greatest challenges because they are more likely to encounter this group than any other
professional; unfortunately, courses designed to study psychopathy are not a traditional
part of basic law enforcement curriculum or for social, behavioural, and forensic profes-
sionals (Herve & Yuille, 2007). The psychopathy literature strongly suggests that it will
often be necessary for police detectives to interview suspects with psychopathic charac-
teristics, and therefore, an understanding of these characteristics and how they may impact
investigations would be valuable to police interviewers and those who advise them
(Quayle, 2008). Not understanding the psychopathic nature of an offender can result in a

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profi ling
J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
Published online 25 November 2010 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/jip.128

*Correspondence to: Frank S. Perri, County of Winnebago, State of Illinois, PO Box 5411, Rockford, IL
61125, USA.
E-mail: frankperri@hotmail.com

42 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)

DOI: 10.1002/jip

fl awed interview strategy that can compromise the strength of a homicide case and result
in a miscarriage of justice at trial (Perri & Lichtenwald, 2008).

One of the most devastating pieces of evidence that law enforcement has the ability to
obtain from suspects is a statement they make concerning an offence. Obtaining statements
are crucial because they reveal motives, their emotional state or lack of one, facts incon-
sistent with the physical evidence, and factual implausibility even though they do not
confess to a crime. A question that is being raised is whether the traditional methods of
interviewing are applicable to psychopathic suspects, or whether there needs to be more
fl exibility in developing a strategy on approaching suspects that at times goes against the
grain of commonly used techniques because traditional techniques may actually be coun-
terproductive to the goal of collecting information.

Often, traditional methods encourage that law enforcement control the interview through
certain techniques that are taught at interview/interrogation seminars. However, if the
interviewer does not understand whom he or she is interviewing and apply a one formula
fi ts all technique, the probability of obtaining useful information may decrease especially
with a psychopathic suspect. An interview cannot be reduced to a singular formula because
people are just not predictable; at best, one can learn a variety of strategies and techniques
that can be used. It is up to the interviewer to analyse a situation and to decide what
strategy is most appropriate, and part of this assessment understands that psychopathic
suspects have different behavioural traits that propel them to engage in conversation dif-
ferently than the interviewer may be used to.

In this paper, important portions of the interview of Christopher Porco who used an axe
to kill his father and attempted to murder the mother are analysed to illustrate how a
traditional interviewing format may produce no confession or important homicide facts.
Case facts are outlined to give the readership an understanding of the antisocial behaviours
that pre-dated the murder that reveal the traits of a psychopath. An overview of psychopa-
thy is outlined to illustrate what these traits are composed of and why they are important
for law enforcement to understand. Understanding the traits are important because in many
respects, they guide the interviewer’s strategy of whether to take a traditional approach or
whether they may have to modify their strategy to take into account the behavioural traits
of a psychopath and how the suspect would react to a particular type of interview.

It should be emphasized that the goal is not to determine if the interviewee is in fact a
bona fi de psychopath because a qualifi ed professional administering the Hare Psychopathic
Checklist Revised (PCL-R) would be required to assess whether the suspect can be
classifi ed a psychopath for clinical purposes. Rather the goal is to consider whether the
interviewee displays psychopathic traits that will assist the interviewer in determining what
interview strategy should be broached to insure that valuable information can be gathered
from an individual who could potentially be a psychopath.

What is traditionally defi ned as a successful interview by law enforcement may have
to be modifi ed without any diminishment to the goals of the interview; it is simply an
alternative method to collect information without necessarily jeopardising the strength of
the case. The importance of the interview is not just in the collection of facts. One must
keep in mind that the interview may be the only time a jury may have to view a defend-
ant’s personality for truthfulness, arrogance, lack of emotions, selfi shness, and manipula-
tion especially if the defendant decides not to testify and all there is is a videotaped
interview. These are the intangible qualities of an interview that have nothing to do with
the evidence per se but with how human nature interprets certain behaviour that speaks
volumes to a jury.

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 43

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

HOMICIDE CASE FACTS

During the early morning hours of November 15, 2004, Christopher Porco, then 21 years
old and a student at the University of Rochester, entered his family home in Delmar, NY
where he brutally murdered his father, Peter, and attempted to murder his mother, Joan,
both with an axe while sleeping (Perri & Lichtenwald, 2007). When the authorities found
her, they found her brain exposed, her jaw had been dislodged, and she eventually lost
one of her eyes. The study of the behavioural data yielded warning signs of Christopher’s
psychopathic qualities. Specifi c examples of this deceitful behaviour include that Chris-
topher was known to have manipulated college transcripts from a local community college,
refl ecting false grades so he could be readmitted to the University of Rochester after he
was forced to withdraw for poor grades (Karlin, 2006). In addition, during the course of
the murder investigation, authorities determined that Christopher had a history of antiso-
cial behaviour that included burglarising his parent’s home, stealing and selling their
computer equipment on eBay.

Interestingly, 1 month prior to the murder, the parents froze their eBay accounts after
Christopher never sent the items sold on eBay; during the investigation, it was revealed
that Christopher posed as his brother sending emails to customers explaining that his
brother had died and was unable to send the purchased items. He was also known to have
broken into his former employer’s place of work stealing cellphones, cameras, and com-
puters. He fraudulently obtained loans using his parents as co-signers without parental
knowledge after obtaining his father’s relevant personal and tax information. Christopher
had told his parents that he only needed a co-signed $2,000 loan for school, yet he fraudu-
lently took out a loan for more than $30,000 (McNiff & Cuomo, 2006). Moreover, to keep
up with the appearance that he was from a wealthy family, he accumulated more than
$40,000 in debt from lavish spending and Internet gambling (McNiff & Cuomo, 2006).

The parents eventually confronted Christopher on his fraudulent behaviour and threat-
ened to go to the authorities to take action against him. In one email, the father wrote:
‘Did you forge my signature as a co-signer? What the hell are you doing? You should have
called me to discuss it . . . I’m calling Citibank this morning to fi nd out what you have
done and am going to tell them I’m not to be on it as co-signor’ (Perri & Lichtenwald,
2008). Citibank notifi ed Peter Porco that Christopher had also obtained a line of credit
over $16,000 to purchase a new Jeep Wrangler (McNiff & Cuomo, 2006). Again, Chris-
topher used his father’s name as co-signatory to secure the auto loan. The parents tried to
contact Christopher via the phone, but Christopher would not talk to them. In another
email, the father wrote: ‘I want you to know that if you abuse my credit again, I will be
forced to fi le forgery affi davits in order to disclaim liability and that applies to the Citibank
college loan if you attempt to reactivate it or use my credit to obtain any other loan’
(McNiff & Cuomo, 2006). Less than 2 weeks from the time the father warned Christopher
that he would not hesitate going to the authorities for his son’s fraudulent behaviour,
Christopher executed his plan to negate the threat.

Several Albany, NY area psychologists and mental health professionals familiar with
the case stated that ‘Christopher Porco fi ts the profi le of a psychopath’ (Grondahl, 2006).
They focused on his continued pattern of lying and deceitful behaviour. Furthermore, these
professionals also pointed to his pattern of grandiose perceptions centring on himself as
a member of a wealthy and infl uential family. Christopher was known to have lied to
friends and acquaintances about a fi ctitious inheritance from his grandmother of millions
of dollars. Whilst awaiting trial, many found him to be strangely arrogant, drinking in

44 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

bars, attending concerts, going out to entertainment establishments, and driving around
the yellow Jeep that witnesses state was the Jeep used to drive to his parent’s home on
the night of the murder. This behaviour fi ts the traits of an individual in need of grandiosity
and embellishment in his life, which is one of the psychopathic traits (Grondahl, 2006).
As forensic psychologist Wulfert stated, ‘There’s an overlap between psychopathic and
narcissistic tendencies . . . He (Christopher) believes that the rules do not apply to him and
he has a need to show off in front of people’ (Grondahl, 2006).

It is interesting to note that during an interview with CBS, while visiting his mother at
the hospital, Christopher stated, ‘I saw her—she was swollen and covered in tubes and
my reaction was I burst into tears. I fell on the fl oor right there’ (Bell, 2007). Yet, a former
youth leader minister named Joseph Catalano who went to the hospital to be with Chris-
topher claimed to be ‘struck by Christopher’s odd behavior because he did not seem to
exhibit any grief’ (Bell, 2007). The psychopaths’ use of the ‘chameleon strategy’ is not
surprising in that they attempt to read a situation and determine what the appropriate
emotional response others want to hear to make them appear sympathetic to outsiders
(Perri & Lichtenwald, 2007). Psychopaths learn how to mimic emotions by watching how
others behave given a set of circumstances (Meloy, 2000).

PSYCHOPATHY OVERVIEW

According to Dr Robert Hare, international psychopathy expert from the University of
British Colombia, the term or concept of ‘psychopathy’ has had a long and sometimes
confusing history. Dr Hare states part of the conceptual confusion stems from the use of
multiple terms to describe similar personality traits and behavioural patterns (e.g. moral
insanity, psychopathic personality, sociopathy, antisocial personality) (Hare, 1993). The
concept of psychopathy refers to a specifi c cluster of traits and behaviours used to describe
an individual in terms of pervasive dominating personality traits and behaviours (Perri &
Lichtenwald, 2007). Whilst psychopathy has similarities to antisocial personality disorder,
which is characterised by a disregard for societal rules including criminal behaviour, it is
not synonymous with or should be confused with criminality or violence in general.
However, those that have psychopathic traits are more at risk for committing crime and
acting out violently (Herve & Yuille, 2007).

According to clinical neuropsychologist and Associate Professor of Clinical Psychiatry
and Clinical Neurology at Northwestern University Dr. Robert Hanlon, psychopathy is a
personality disorder that can also be considered a mental disorder, but it is not considered
to be as functionally disabling as a more commonly known mental illness such as schizo-
phrenia (Hanlon, 2010). Personality disorders such as psychopathy are not comparable to
mental illnesses that display psychotic disorder traits where there is irrational and disor-
ganized thinking resulting in not being able to appreciate the criminality of their actions
(Hanlon, 2010). Moreover most psychopaths are mentally capable of appreciating the
criminality of their actions and can be rather methodical and strategic regarding their
crimes even though they may display an impulsive lifestyle (Hanlon, 2010) characterized
by thrill seeking activities, a parasitic lifestyle, being irresponsible and displaying anti-
social traits (Hare, 1993). Personality disorders can be characterised by a class of personal-
ity types that deviate from societal expectations of what is appropriate behaviour. Research
has proven that those that have personality disorders display a rigidity or infl exibility in
their thinking, feeling, and behaviours that impairs them from functioning with others in

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 45

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

a larger societal context. In contrast to personality disorders, mental illness is characterised
by a probability of a biochemical imbalance that may act as a catalyst for the individual
to behave in an inexplicable, erratic behaviour that has no connection to logic such as
cause and effect. Psychopaths, however, are more likely than non-psychopaths to feign
mental illness and to engage in deception during insanity evaluations (Hakkanen-Nyholm
& Hare, 2009). It is a common belief amongst law enforcement and forensic professionals
that people who commit violent incomprehensible crimes must be crazy, psychotic, or they
‘just snapped’ (Herve & Yuille, 2007). Moreover, the fact that an individual may have a
mental illness does not mean that they cannot also be psychopathic; they are not mutually
exclusive and do co-occur (Murphy & Vess, 2003).

Unfortunately, many are capable of fooling professionals who observe ‘abnormal behav-
iour’ and equate it to a mental illness issue when the same professionals ignore the calcu-
lating, manipulating, and planning beneath their schemes that are not the symptoms of
someone who is mentally ill. Dr Hare described psychopaths as an intra-species predator
that uses charm, manipulation, intimidation, and violence to control others and to satisfy
their own selfi sh needs. Lacking in conscience and in feelings for others, they cold-
bloodedly take what they want and do as they please, violating social norms and expec-
tations without the slightest sense of guilt or regret (Hare, 1993). Dr Hare notes that
psychopathy can be distinguished from offi cially recognised personality disorders, ‘. . . on
the basis of its characteristic pattern of interpersonal, affective and behavioral symptoms’,
(Hare, 1993). Some of the major personality and behavioural traits identifi ed by Hare are
noted in the table in the next section (Herve & Yuille, 2007) (Table 1).

Psychopaths are not disoriented or out of touch with reality, nor do they experience the
delusions, hallucinations, or intense subjective distress that characterises most other mental
disorders. They are rational and aware of what they are doing and why. Their behaviour
is the result of choice, freely exercised but coupled with a distorted sense of reality (Perri
& Lichtenwald, 2007). As Edelgard Wulfert, forensic psychologist and professor at the
University of New York at Albany, stated, ‘A psychopath invents reality to conform to his
needs’ (Grondahl, 2006). Psychopaths also have diffi culty projecting into the future, that
is, understanding how their actions play themselves out in life and they also have defi cits
in refl ecting upon their past; ‘[t]hey are prisoners of the present’ (Meloy, 2000).

Psychopathy appears to be one of the strongest predictors of aggression and violence
and the distinct psychopathic traits of lack of empathy and lack of remorse are the best
indicators of aggression especially in unprovoked aggression (Reidy, Zeichner, & Mar-
tinez, 2008a). Motives for psychopathic aggression may include the need to control situ-
ations/persons, hate of others, and restoring the false narcissistic self that has been damaged
by criticism or threats by others (Martens, 2003). Expanding on Martens (2003), research
has shed light on the fact that the narcissistic sub-dimension of psychopathy is linked to

Table 1. Psychopathic personality traits

Interpersonal Affective Lifestyle Antisocial

Superfi cial charm Remorselessness Impulsivity Poor behavioral controls
Grandiosity Shallow affect Stimulation seeking Delinquency
Lying Callousness Irresponsible Criminal versatility
Conning and

manipulative
Failure to accept

responsibility
Parasitic lifestyle
Lack of realistic goals

Early behavioral
problems

46 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

the probability that a psychopath will resort to violence (Cale & Lilienfeld, 2006) and
those that displayed traits of extreme entitlement and exploitation of others to achieve their
goals were more likely to resort to extreme forms of aggression and deleterious violence
against innocent people even in the absence of provocation (Reidy, Zeichner, Foster, &
Martinez, 2008b).

Psychopaths tend to engage in violence, especially homicide, in a more predatory,
instrumental manner and are willing to take their time to plan the kill as contrasted to
non-psychopathic killers (Herve & Yuille, 2007). The absence of emotion actually assists
them in the planning the kill and not killing reactively because a time requirement for
predation is not necessarily present (Meloy, 2000). For a homicide to be rated as instru-
mental, the offence had to have been clearly goal-oriented in nature with no evidence of
an immediate emotional or situational provocation; the motive for the homicide has to be
for something other than hot-blooded, spontaneous anger, or frustrated behaviour and that
is what is illustrated in these cases (Woodworth & Porter, 2002). Psychopaths’ overwhelm-
ingly engage in instrumental violence when committing murder by almost a two-to-one
margin over non-psychopaths and they did not display a state of heightened emotional
arousal at the time of the murder as contrasted to non-psychopaths whose murders exhib-
ited an emotional discharge such as ‘jealousy, rage or a heated argument during the offense’
(Woodworth & Porter, 2002). Thus, the rage displayed by a psychopath should not be
confused with the emotion-based rage that Woodworth and Porter refer to and that law
enforcement erroneously concludes when they do not have any insights into the behav-
ioural profi le of a suspect. Quite the opposite holds true; the psychopath’s display of rage
in the context of instrumental violence represents a dispassionate expression of their
devaluation of others (Perri & Lichtenwald, 2010) where murder is a viable solution to
satisfy their motives (Woodworth & Porter, 2002).

Furthermore, although psychopathic impulsivity that is displayed in Table 1 under the
heading ‘Lifestyle’ can mean ‘unpremeditated’, ‘acting before thinking’ or the ‘spur of the
moment’ behavior as well as parasitic behaviors, being irresponsible and thrill seeking,
one should not extrapolate this to mean that somehow psychopathic aggression is always
random and lacking in refl ection. Woodworth and Porter’s (2002) research on the instru-
mental nature of psychopathic homicidal violence is further supported by Dr. Hanlon’s
observations and clinical experience assessing psychopathic homicidal offenders that their
violence can be methodical and strategic. The fact that psychopathic criminals get caught
should not necessarily be interpreted as the result of a ‘spur of the moment’ decision to
offend or lack of planning because of an impulsive behavioral disposition.

In contrast, for reactive violence to be present, there must be strong evidence for a high
level of spontaneity/impulsivity and a lack of planning surrounding the commission of the
offence; thus, a rapid and powerful affective reaction prior to the act with no apparent goal
other than to harm the victim immediately following a provocation/confl ict (Woodworth
& Porter, 2002). Reactive violence is more illustrative between family members and
acquaintances, whilst instrumental violence is more illustrative of violence between stran-
gers, but what is interesting in the Porco case is that it was instrumental violence and not
reactive violence that was used to murder family (Woodworth & Porter, 2002). Because
their violence is often instrumental and committed without intense emotion, they would
be less distraught and immobilised with fear or confusion in post-offence behaviour
(Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009). Post-offence behaviour is an important indicator of
whether psychopathy traits are present and that interviewers cannot ignore in formulating
an interview strategy.

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 47

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

THE INTERVIEW

Despite little empirical research on the interviewing of psychopathic suspects and offend-
ers, many criminal investigators are well aware of the issues and problems that they have
in working with these persons. Law enforcement has shown interest in psychopathy for
understanding criminal behaviour particularly in criminal investigation, crime scene analy-
sis, and interviewing (Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009). Law enforcement faces a formi-
dable challenge attempting to evaluate explanations by these persons, and the task is even
more daunting when it has to deal with the glib, grandiose, and egocentric individuals who
are usually adept at dissimulation, blame externalisation, and who are not embarrassed or
shaken at being caught in a lie (Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009).

The Porco case is an important illustration of how not to conduct an interview and the
transcript of the interview should be used as a teaching and training tool by law enforce-
ment agencies (Transcript of Christopher Porco’s Interrogation, 2004). Too often, violent
crime investigators attempt to overwhelm the suspect by playing into his or her emotional
mindset to obtain a confession whilst neglecting preparation and planning of an interview
because of the belief that a suspect’s distraught emotional state of mind will not make
interview preparation necessary. Approaching interviews with suspects that display psy-
chopathic traits by attempting to appeal to an empathic format may prove to be fatally
fl awed. When interviewing a potentially psychopathic suspect, however, this not only
means ensuring great familiarity with the case under investigation (i.e. all evidence, ante-
cedent history, and current condition of the suspect) and the strategy to be employed (with
specifi c objectives) but also anticipating how the interviewee is likely to behave in response
to aspects of the strategy or lines of questioning (Quayle, 2008). This aspect of the inter-
view was woefully lacking in the Porco case, and it is apparent in the interviewer’s frus-
tration when they realised that trying to emotionally overwhelm Christopher did not work
and they had no alternative strategy to implement.

Using an emotion-based approach might be effective with a non-psychopathic suspect;
however, it probably is not transferable to a psychopathic suspect who does not have the
ability to bond with others who produce emotional responses. Throughout the interview,
the investigators elicited answers that to a trained interviewer would have revealed psy-
chopathic traits and would have accordingly given the interviewer time to gauge a strategy
to approach the suspect differently. For example, during the interview, Porco admitted that
he was impulsive, irresponsible, a liar, had an infl ated view of himself, engaged in gran-
diosity, and enjoyed impressing others with fi ctitious facts coupled with those intangible
qualities such as showing no emotion about the death of a parent just hours before the
interview. It is suggested that an understanding of how psychopathic characteristics can
manifest themselves in criminal behaviour and during investigative interviews is important
to those advising on police interviewing and to those conducting interviews with suspects
(Quayle, 2008).

Psychopathic suspects believe that they are capable of creating factual scenarios that
others will accept because of their embellished views of their manipulative abilities. Even
though they may be talking to a trained investigator does not diminish their deceitful
ways, and in fact, they may enjoy the interview and experience ‘duping delight’ (Hare,
1993). The fact that these offenders will not be truthful is not as important as getting these
suspects to talk (Perri & Lichtenwald, 2008). However, interviewers must be aware that
interviewing the psychopath may prove to be challenging if the interview is not advanced
with the goal of eliciting inconsistent, implausible information as opposed to getting the

48 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

suspect to speak the truth. According to interviewing expert Donald Rabon, ‘Confronting
the pathological person with facts and details that are contrary to his assertions is like
nailing Jell-o to a wall’ (Rabon, 2006). When dealing with the pathological interviewee,
give him the facts and details that will refute his assertions, but do not expect him to
acquiesce or recant previous statements. Furthermore, even if the suspect is confronted
with evidence that contradicts his assertions, do not expect the pathological suspect to
display anxiety or emotional discomfort (Rabon, 2006). Yet, by getting the psychopath to
talk, a trail of statements that are contradictory to undisputed facts coupled with a lack of
proper emotional affect produce a powerful image of deception and lack credibility at
trial.

Just as interviewers modify their interviewing style when speaking to minors, the same
fl exibility is required when confronting psychopathic suspects. Psychopathic characteris-
tics may be suggested in a suspect’s mode of interacting with others, his history and
manner of offending, and in other information concerning his lifestyle, relationships,
educational, and employment history, etc. (Quayle, 2008). It is crucial for an investigator
to attempt to observe whether there are psychopathic traits because the assessment will
guide the strategy, and if the strategy is fl awed, it is highly probable that important infor-
mation will not be gathered as evidenced by the Porco interview. Moreover, if the inter-
viewer observes that his or her strategy is not working because he or she did not take the
psychopathic traits into account when a strategy was implemented, the suspect may see
through new strategies and possibly refuse to co-operate with the interview and invoke
the right to counsel, which is exactly what happened in the Porco interview.

Many non-psychopathic murder defendants might break down emotionally because they
experience a sense of remorse. Yet, this was not observable in the Porco case. Assuming
that it was not to late to do so, once the interviewers suspected that Christopher had strong
characteristics of a psychopath, their strategy should have refl ected a non-confrontational
approach. One observes the interviewers attempting to turn ‘up the heat’ on Porco with a
barrage of questions aimed to break him down emotionally so that he will confess to the
murder. Though the questions and answers outlined here do not refl ect the order in which
they were asked, they illustrate how the investigator’s strategy of attempting to overwhelm
him emotionally was not as effi cient as it could have been.

Detective: Listen to me. It’s not—it’s not—it’s a crime of passion, okay? Like an
emotional thing. You know, that’s what it is.
Porco: That’s not the (inaudible one word)—
Detective: An emotional fl are up or something, you know, maybe. That’s what I’m
looking for. Give me something to grasp here. Let me get through the night.
Porco: Nothing happened.
Detective: And you understand what I’ve been telling you, that this is not a robbery.
This is a crime of emotions.
Porco: I know what you are telling me. I—I don’t know. I have absolutely no idea.
Detective: I mean, like I told you, in my estimation, that situation, the way I’m
seeing it, was something that happened out of a passionate moment.
Porco: You told me they were in bed, so I don’t know how passionate that could be,
honestly.
Detective: And then afterwards, those emotions subside and the thought is, what’s
happened? What have I done? What has happened here? What an awful thing.
Porco: I agree it’s an awful thing.

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 49

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

Detective: Sometimes your emotions get the best of you, overtake you.
Porco: True.
Detective: Your stomach is going to burn a hole in it. The only way to stop it is to
be a man.
Porco: I can’t help you.

The investigator attempted to play into Porco’s emotions and manhood as a strategy to
elicit a confession, but the strategy got him nowhere. Interviewers must avoid belittling
the psychopathic suspect (Quayle, 2008). Porco gives bland answers, absent of emotion;
neither do his responses offer any insight into the murder itself. Instead of attempting to
get him to feel guilty about his actions in the hope of a confession, the investigator should
have remained calm, similar to Porco, to ask questions. When Porco’s responses did not
match the evidence collected, the investigator should have—calmly—confronted Porco
about the inconsistency and further allowed him the opportunity to bury himself with
more lies that could be revealed at trial together with his lack of emotions just hours after
he killed his father. The investigator incorrectly transferred a non-psychopathic explana-
tion onto the murder by attempting to inure an emotion to the killing to suggest that
somehow Porco’s anger was the impetus for the killing. The investigators failed to under-
stand that the issue was not about anger or any other emotion but about using murder as
a solution to a problem. Another error made by law enforcement in developing an inter-
view strategy is that they equate the horrifi c manner in which a murder was performed
to the amount of anger or rage the offender experienced at the time of the act. Inter-
viewers must not project their emotions to the disturbing nature of the offence (Quayle,
2008).

In addition another mistake that many in law enforcement make when they learn of a
homicide that is between family and/or acquaintances is that the offender must have been
angry by the intense manner they appear to have killed; yet, the intense manner of the
murder may or may not have anything to do with the behavioural composition of the
offender, who may not inure any emotion to the murder (Perri & Lichtenwald, 2008).
The fact that their murders appear to be reactive, which is with sudden and intense passion,
does not mean that the intense manner of their murder should be linked to an emotion; in
fact, research indicates the exact opposite when psychopathy’s link to homicide is exam-
ined (Herve & Yuille, 2007). Psychopaths who overwhelmingly commit instrumental
violence do not display a state of heightened emotional arousal at the time of the murder
as contrasted to non-psychopaths whose reactive murders exhibit an emotional discharge
such as ‘jealousy, rage or a heated argument during the offense’ (Woodworth & Porter,
2002). Thus, the rage displayed by a psychopath should not be confused with the emotion-
based rage that Woodworth and Porter referred to and that law enforcement erroneously
concludes when they observe crime scenes. Quite the opposite holds true, the psychopath’s
rage is a dispassionate display of conscienceless behaviour to control others. Because they
lack empathy, do not have the ability to anticipate remorse coupled with devaluation of
others explain why psychopaths partake in dispassionate instrumental violence given their
motive to kill is not necessarily emotionally driven (Meloy, 2002).

Moreover, given that psychopathic violence is often instrumental and committed
without intense emotion, they would be less distraught and not immobilised with fear or
confusion in post-offence behaviour (Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009). This research of
post-offence behaviour is extremely important for law enforcement to consider when
evaluating whether the suspect displays psychopathic traits or not. This post-behaviour

50 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

observation of lack of trauma, lack of affect is clearly evident when Christopher wanted
to talk to law enforcement. In fact, the videotaped interrogation of Porco recorded within
less than 24 hours of the homicide illustrates a controlled manner, never wept for the
news he received about his parents condition yet desired to want to engage the police
to see what information they had that revealed evidence of his culpability (Perri &
Lichtenwald, 2008).

About midway through the interview, a detective from the New York State Police was
brought in. He was very matter of fact in his inquiry and non-threatening in his approach.
He questioned Porco about his trait of grandiosity.

Detective: I’m still not quite understanding why you go around telling people that
you have all this money. That kind of baffl es me.
Porco: No, I—
Detective:—you put on this façade.
Porco: It is. And I’ve done it for a couple of years now.
Detective: What does that say about your character—
Porco: Exactly.
Detective:—Chris? Seriously. What—no tell me, what does it say about your character?
Porco: I guess it’s insecurity in a way.
Detective: Does it tell you that you’re an honest person?
Porco: Not in that respect, no.
Detective: Trustworthy?
Porco: I think of myself as trustworthy, I guess. If we’re going to respond in refer-
ence to this, no, I guess I’m not.
Detective: I mean, if I—if I’m a student and I meet you and you’re telling me you’re
going to buy a house because you’ve got millions of dollars from your grandmother.
Porco: True.
Detective: You’re basically lying to these people, right?
Porco: Well, it kind of steamrolled away.
Detective: No, let’s not color code it.
Porco: I know it’s wrong.
Detective: Do you consider it lying?
Porco: Oh, defi nitely, yeah. You know, I’m telling them something that’s not really
true.
Detective: Are you a pathological liar?
Porco: I don’t think so. I mean, maybe in that respect.
Detective: What other things have you lied about?
Porco: Really one thing. I’m trying to think of anything else. Just money. I don’t
really know why I got started on it. It was in high school.

This non-confrontational style the detective should have continued with Porco. The
detective did not have to browbeat Porco into the admission that he lies. Yet, as the inter-
view progressed, he, too, fell into the same strategy the other detectives followed, which
was to use an emotional and confrontational approach. The interview also failed because
the detectives did not understand the workings of fraud to know how to adequately ques-
tion him about the white collar crimes that predated the murder, which would have
revealed a fraud detection homicide motive where one kills to silence those that are in a
position to detect and disclose their fraud schemes to the authorities (Brody & Kiehl, 2010;

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 51

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

Perri & Lichtenwald, 2007). Perhaps, in the planning stage of the interview, the detectives
could have included a detective who has experience in fraud to be part of the interview
team if the evidence suggests that fraud detection by the parents was the motive for the
murder. Although other types of physical evidence may assist in developing possible
suspects, professionals who understand fraud may be in a unique position to uncover
a motive that the physical evidence does not reveal. A white-collar crime detective
could have inquired into the fraud that Christopher downplayed. For example, what was
Christopher’s reaction when he learned that his father would go to the authorities? Why
did he buy a new Jeep when Christopher indicated that he borrowed money for school
expenses? Although such questions had nothing to do with the murder, the answers might
have displayed volumes about his character that the jury could consider and a possible
motive the prosecution could advance.

As one can observe, Christopher revealed traits that would have alerted the interviewers
that he had traits refl ective of a psychopath and that a non-traditional interview approach
should be considered as an initial strategy (Table 2).

MODIFICATION OF THE INTERVIEW STRATEGY

Many law enforcement agencies use the Reid Technique as a method of interviewing and
interrogating suspects. Supporters of the technique argue that it helps to extract informa-
tion from unwilling participants. The technique detectives used in the Porco case appears
to be the Reid Technique, and although it may be useful, there are limits to its success
depending on the type of individual being interviewed. The Reid method includes nine
steps of interrogation, but some of these techniques can backfi re when dealing with psy-
chopathic personalities. Techniques are often anecdotally endorsed by those who can point
to success stories; however, there are times when a technique can fail as it did in this
important homicide case. One of the premises of the Reid Technique is that the interviewer
should be in control of the interview, preventing the suspect from denying guilt by cutting
off unsatisfactory explanations and ultimately working towards a confession.

Table 2. Interviewee psychopathic trait identifi cation checklist

Psychopathy traits Christopher’s responses

Superfi cial charm Complements that the detectives on doing a good job.
Egocentric Christopher wanted to know why the police took so long in calling him

about participating in the interview.
No affect/emotion There was no emotional display by Christopher of knowing his father is

dead or mother; his reaction at the hospital observing his mother and
none at the interview just hours after Peter’s murder.

Lack of remorse Christopher displayed no signs of remorse during the interview.
Pathological lying Christopher admitted on several occasion to lying to family and friends.
Manipulative Christopher took advantage of family members and former employer,

forging school transcripts.
Grandiosity Christopher bragged about coming from a wealthy family, which was false.
Impulsivity Christopher admitted to being impulsive.
Poor planning Christopher admitted that he does not know what happens to his fi nances

in terms of not having money available and does not know or care about
how his behavior contributes to his fi nancial problems.

Criminal behavior Police knew of his history of theft and fraud.

52 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

The interrogator offers two contrasting motives for some aspect of the crime. One sug-
gested motive might appear to be socially acceptable, such as to call an act ‘a crime
of passion’ and then contrasting to a morally unacceptable motive suggesting that ‘one
kills for the money’. The detective builds up the contrast between the two alternatives
until the suspect gives an indication that he has chosen one. In the Porco case, the detec-
tives initially gave Chris the option to choose the crime of passion motive for the kill that
produced no confession. Next, the detectives pitched the morally repugnant motive for the
murder.

Detective: Let me guess, there’s an insurance policy?
Porco: I would kill my parents for money. Brilliant.
Detective: Chris, you have no alibi. Do you understand that?
Porco: Inaudible statement.
Detective: You have a motive.
Porco: I don’t understand what you think my motive is.
Detective: Well, money.
Porco: I’m going to kill my parents for $300,000? I mean, come on.
Detective: Yeah.
Porco: Why?
Detective: People do it all the time.
Porco: That’s horrible.
Detective: You know what I’m saying? You got to get this off your chest . . . This is
going to eat you up. You know that, don’t you? Your hair is going to fall out.
Porco: I can’t help you.

The problem with attempting to confront Chris with these two different motives is that
neither motive is applicable or matters to Christopher. Specifi cally, Christopher does not
reactively kill because of an argument he had with his father or mother nor is Christopher
bothered by the morally repugnant reason to prove motive because his pathological narcis-
sism refl ects a highly entitled, highly exploitive person. He does not have a problem with
taking advantage of others for self-gratifi cation reasons nor does he engage in a moral
debate about the rightness or wrongness of a given behaviour. In cases where the inter-
viewer suspects that he or she is in the presence of a true psychopath, it is reasonable to
begin the interview using tactics appropriate for a psychopathic suspect. If the interviewer
begins using a strategy more appropriate for a non-psychopath, upon realising that the
strategy is not working, he or she may not be able to switch strategies midway through
questioning the suspect.

What is clear about this interview is that Christopher wanted to participate in the inter-
view and that should have been a clue to the interviewers of the type of person they are
dealing with when Christopher complained about why it took the police so long to call
him to have him participate in an interview. Christopher already knew that he was under
no obligation to co-operate because the law protects one against self-incrimination, thus
Christopher did not have to wait to be interviewed to invoke his right to remain silent; he
could have refused to speak with the police when he was asked to come in for questioning.
In addition, Christopher’s father was a lawyer who understood criminal law and procedure
and probably discussed these concepts with his family even if on an informal basis. For
example, when the police start becoming aggressive in their approach in trying to over-
whelm him, he invoked his Miranda Rights, which meant that the police had to cease

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 53

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

questioning. Yet, we see even after he invokes his rights, the detectives continue to ques-
tion him because of their frustration.

Detective: Listen, all right. I’ve known you for a long time? For a long time, all
right? I’m not going to ask you anything. We’ll take a ride and let’s try to straighten
this out, okay? We’re going to go up to—we’ll go and take a polygraph and make
sure you’re—at least exonerate you.
Porco: Do I have to have a lawyer present for that?
Detective: No, Well, at least it will exonerate you from—from this.
Porco: Right. I want to have a lawyer present for that.
Detective: Why is that?
Porco: Because I’m not sure that I’m being treated fairly.
Detective: You are being treated fairly.
Porco: Well, I’d like to make sure of that.
Detective: Well, if—are you accepting to take the polygraph? Are you accepting to
take it?
Porco: I will take one.
Detective: M-m h-m-m.
Porco: After I’ve spoken to a lawyer.
Detective: Why do you need to speak to one fi rst?
Porco: Inaudible statement.
Detective: Well, look it, Chris. Why would that be?
Porco: Because I want to make sure that my rights are protected. I feel I have done
nothing wrong.
Detective: Well, I’ll tell you what. I’m not going to run the test.
Porco: I’m not saying you’re biased. I don’t think you are. I—
Detective: No we don’t do that test.
Porco: I hope you don’t. I know you don’t.
Detective: Well, look it. I’m just going to throw this at you real quick. I had a rape
case some years ago, all right? The person went in and took the polygraph—
Porco: M-m h-m-m.
Detective:—And passed it. I shook his hand and bid him a good day. I’m serious.
I’m dead serious right? Afterwards, I found out through further investigation, in fact,
he was innocent, all right?
Porco: That’s good.
Detective: This is what I’m telling you. Keep-us like I said, give us a baseline here.
Porco: After I’ve spoken to a lawyer, I’d be happy to take one.
Detective: Well, I’m going to tell you this too. No lawyer I’ve ever met has allowed
their client to take a polygraph, and I’ll tell you why. It’s because they feel as though
it’s never going to be good for them. If you’re innocent though, it will indicate that.
I’m telling you right now it does indicate false or true.
Porco: Okay. I know they work very well.
Detective: We don’t do it. So, that’s why I would hope that you would take it just
to eliminate you, please.
Porco: After I have spoken to a lawyer, I will be happy to take one.
Detective: As I said, I don’t know of one—
Porco: I’ll talk to a lawyer about it and make a decision. I don’t feel I have anything
to hide, but I—

54 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

Detective: Well you know what? If I felt as though it was still incriminating of
evidence I have right now, I wouldn’t be asking you to take it because I wouldn’t
need it. You know what I mean?
Porco: I understand that.
Detective: Okay. All right.
Porco: I would hope there wouldn’t be. I honestly believe that if my mother
told you that I killed her husband, you would already have—you’d have me in
handcuffs.
Detective: No—I’m dead serious.

Again, we see Porco agreeing to take a polygraph with a lawyer present and the detec-
tive questioning him about why he wants an attorney. We also observe how much bolder
Christopher becomes with the detective on criminal matters. Initially, during the ‘get to
know you stage’ Chris was more subdued, but once he called the detectives’ bluff, his
egocentrism could not be contained. Although the entire 6-hour transcript cannot be tran-
scribed, the unfortunate aspect is that the detectives offered little in terms of sophistication,
no indication of a planned interview and how to conduct follow-up questions with infor-
mation Christopher disclosed. They appeared chaotic, spoke in vague generalities and were
no match for Christopher who pressed the detectives for information as the interview
progresses. What was observed are detectives constantly being apologetic to Christopher
and trying to win Christopher’s approval of who they are and then switching strategies by
trying to make him feel guilty about what he did. Police interviewers are often drawn into
answering questions that, in the eyes of a psychopath, diminish their credibility or author-
ity, and this is evident in this case (Quayle, 2008).

It is the culmination of the lies that eventually expose a suspect’s culpability. It is easy
to explain away a certain number of inconsistencies in statements they disclose during
an interview. Yet, how many innocent explanations can explain away the inconsistencies
and deceit? Visualise the jury hearing that he forged his transcripts, forged his father’s
signature, lied to his friends about money he never had trying to impress people by
telling people that his grandmother owned property in Connecticut, which was false,
stole from his parents, and did not shed a tear for his parents during the interview. Keep
in mind that these facts have nothing to do with the homicide facts. Imagine in the Porco
case if a jury was allowed to view 6 hours of illogical, manipulative, deceitful, inconsist-
ent, and implausible testimony from the defendant. Furthermore, if a defendant decides
to testify on his or her own behalf, those statements can be used to further impeach his
or her credibility in the event he or she gives further inconsistent testimony in court. In
court, the defendant is not in a position to avoid those he or she believes he or she can
not manipulate. If a defendant is willing to lie about the most mundane and innocuous
facts, what is he or she willing to lie about when it comes to important facts about a
murder?

Unfortunately the prosecution was not allowed to disclose to the jury Christopher’s
statements made during the interview or display the videotape that revealed his emotion-
less, deceitful and manipulative qualities for the jury to consider in their determination
of guilt or innocence. The trial court correctly determined the police had violated Chris-
topher’s Miranda Rights in that the police continued to question him about the homicide
when he invoked his right to counsel, thus his statement could not be used as evidence
against him nor could the prosecution comment on those intangible qualities, such as his
demeanor, that speak volumes to a jury.

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 55

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

CONCLUSION

Many in law enforcement believe that in order to consider the interview a success, it is
imperative to get a confession. The fact that a detective does not get a confession does
not mean that the interview was not a success. The defi nition of what constitutes a suc-
cessful interview may have to be altered for psychopathic suspects nor is shifting strategies
an indication that the interview will be less fruitful; in fact, it increases the probability of
obtaining relevant information and exposing those intangible aspects of an interview such
as how the person reacts when they received news of the homicide. In fact, the inconsistent
and implausible responses the detective elicits from criminals are devastating when dis-
closed in court. Imagine the jury in the Porco case viewing 6 hours of egocentric, emotion-
less, manipulative statements from the defendant.

Furthermore, if a defendant decides to testify on his or her own behalf, those statements
can be used to further impeach his or her credibility in the event he or she gives further
inconsistent testimony during a trial. In court, these defendants are not in a position to
avoid those they can not manipulate. Consider also the fact that defence attorneys attempt
to project an image to the jury that their client was tricked, intimidated, and eventually
coerced into giving a confession to interrogators. This defence strategy is virtually non-
existent as the jury views a videotaped conversation of the defendant speaking freely,
voluntarily, and without coercion or force. Instead, the jury views an investigator simply
collecting facts and giving the suspect the opportunity to tell his or her side of the events
coupled with the fact there was no coerced confession.

Not taking the time to assess a suspect’s personality traits can lead to a fl awed interview
strategy that yields neither a confession nor valuable information. The Christopher Porco
case is an excellent example of how not to conduct an interview, but it does serve as an
excellent training tool for law enforcement to learn from. In addition, given that law
enforcement is more apt to come into contact with psychopaths than other professionals,
it behoves law enforcement training centres to begin the process of training professionals
on traits particular to psychopaths and how to use their traits to their advantage when
conducting an interview.

Strategies to consider when interviewing a psychopathy

• Do not necessarily rely on past strategies that were successful in obtaining confessions
to determine future strategies. Interviews that do not produce a confession are not nec-
essarily unsuccessful if they produce implausible and inconsistent statements that impact
the credibility of a suspect.

• The interview of the suspect can not be based on appeals for sympathy, remorse, regret,
or social obligations (Quayle, 2008). The interview has to be based on a non-emotional
format and the dialogue must revolve around facts and specifi c evidence (Perri &
Lichtenwald, 2008). We observed this failed strategy on Porco.

• The interviewer should avoid projecting their own emotions when considering an inter-
view strategy. We can observe how the detectives in the Porco case projected how
Christopher should feel because they assumed that that is how a person should react to
a horrifi c crime (Quayle, 2008).

• Avoid confronting the suspect with a belittling style observed in the Porco case. (Perri
& Lichtenwald, 2008). Instead, interviews with suspects should be planned and evidence
against the suspect must be collected to confront the suspect when he or she denies the

56 F. S. Perri

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

facts. In the Porco case, the detectives rushed to interview Christopher within hours of
the murder with no evidence to confront him with when he denied his involvement.

• Consider for interview strategy purposes incorporating post-offence behaviours dis-
played by a suspect as a possible indicator of psychopathic traits. Psychopaths are more
likely to not display post-offence traumatic behaviours because murder is more often
instrumental and committed without intense emotion. They are less distraught and
immobilised with fear or confusion (Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009).

• Much of the focus revolves around the interviewee; however, it is important that the
interviewer understands his or her own personality strengths and weaknesses. Know
thyself: is the interviewer an individual that has to be in control of an interview and the
interviewee when in fact it may be necessary for the suspect to believe he or she is in
control?

• Avoid being overly confi dent in believing that the interviewer is capable of detecting
deception. To date, practitioners in the criminal justice system are no better at detecting
deception than the average person, although they are confi dent in their ability to tell
whether someone is lying (Ekman, O’Sullivan, & Frank, 1991, 1999; Ekman, O’Sullivan
& Frank, 1991; Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009).

• If it is evident that the suspect being interviewed is likely the culprit, the interviewer’s
goal is to collect as many inconsistent and implausible facts as possible. Presenting
incriminating evidence to the suspect does not increase the probability of a confession
(Rabon, 2006). When dealing with the pathological interviewee, give him or her the
facts and details that will refute his or her assertions, but do not expect him or her to
necessarily acquiesce or recant previous statements.

• If the suspect does show emotions, the interviewer must be aware that they are emotions
learned from observing how others behave in a given situation (Hare, 1993; Perri,
Lichtenwald, & MacKenzie, 2008). The psychopath may be using this strategy for any
number of reasons (i.e. to test the interviewer’s strategy, evaluate how clever the inter-
viewer is, probe the interviewer for personal weakness).

• Understand the nature of the crime and its victimology. In this case, it was a parricide.
Does the interviewer understand the offender profi le of adult children who kill parents,
possible motives, and crime scene traits? For example, some adult children who kill
their parents do so to avoid having the parents detect crimes perpetrated against
them and potentially disclose them to law enforcement (Heide, 1995). In addition,
some of these adult children are psychopathic (Perri et al., 2008; Weisman & Sharma,
1997).

REFERENCES

Bell, R. (2007). Retrieved October 22, 2010 from http://www.trutv.com/library/crime/notorious_
murders/family/christopher_porco/4.html

Brody, R. G., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010). From white collar to red collar crime. Journal of Financial
Crime, 17(3), 351–364.

Cale, E. M., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2006). Psychopathy factors and risks for aggressive behavior:
A test of the ‘threatened egotism’ hypothesis. Law and Human Behavior, 30(1), 51–74.

Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G. (1991). Who can catch a liar. American Psychologist,
46, 913–920.

Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G. (1999). A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science,
10, 263–266.

The fl awed interview of a psychopathic killer: what went wrong? 57

Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Investig. Psych. Offender Profi l. 8: 41–57 (2011)
DOI: 10.1002/jip

Grondahl, P. (2006). Retrieved October 22, 2010 from http://www.timesunion.com/news/article/
Porco-labeled-psycho-killer-580304.php

Hakkanen-Nyholm, H., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Psychopathy, homicide and the courts: Working the
system. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36(8), 761–777.

Hanlon, R. (2010, October, 19). Correspondence and interview with Dr. Robert Hanlon.
Hare, R. (1993). Without conscience; the disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. New York,

NY: The Guilford Press Inc.
Heide, K. M. (1995). Dangerously antisocial youths who kill their parents. Journal of Police and

Criminal Psychology, 10, 10–14.
Herve, H., & Yuille, J. (2007). The psychopath. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Karlin, R. (2006). Forged transcript center of attention. Retrieved October 22, 2010 from http://

www.timesunion.com/news/article/Forged-transcripts-center-of-attention-580255.php
Martens, W. H. J. (2003). Constructive functions of aggressions in psychopaths. Priory Metical

Journals. Retrieved October 22, 2010 from http://priory.com/psych/socio.htm
McNiff, E., & Cuomo, C. (2006). Amid the carnage, a mother’s love. Retrieved October 22, 2010

from http://abcnews.go.com/US/LegalCenter/story?id=2179489&page=1
Meloy, J. R., (2000). Violent risk and threat assessment: A practical guide for mental health and

criminal justice professionals. San Diego, CA: Specialized Training Services.
Meloy, J. R. (2002). The psychopathic mind. Rowman & Littlefi eld Publishers Inc.: Maryland.
Murphy, C., & Vess, J. (2003). Subtypes of psychopathy: Proposed differences between narcissistic,

borderline, sadistic and antisocial psychopaths. Psychiatric Quarterly, 74(1), 11–29.
Perri, F. S., & Lichtenwald, T. G. (2007). A proposed addition to the FBI criminal classifi cation

manual: Fraud detection homicide. Forensic Examiner, 16(4), 18–31.
Perri, F. S., & Lichtenwald, T. G. (2008). The arrogant chameleons: Exposing fraud detection

homicide. Forensic Examiner, 17(1), 26–34.
Perri, F. S., & Lichtenwald, T. G. (2010). The last frontier: Myths & the female psychopathic killer.

Forensic Examiner, 19(2), 50–67.
Perri, F. S., Lichtenwald, T. G., & MacKenzie, P. (2008). The lull before the storm: Adult children

who kill their parents. Forensic Examiner, 17(3), 40–54.
Quayle, J. (2008). Interviewing a psychopathic suspect. Journal of Investigative Psychology and

Offender Profi ling, 5, 79–91.
Rabon, D. (2006). Interviewing the pathological subject. Fraud Magazine, 20(3), 53–54.
Reidy, D. E., Zeichner, A., & Martinez, M. A. (2008a). Effects of psychopathy traits on unprovoked

aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 319–328.
Reidy, D. E., Zeichner, A., Foster, J. D., & Martinez, M. A. (2008b). Effects of narcissistic entitle-

ment and exploitativeness on human physical aggression. Personality and Individual Differences,
44, 865–875.

Transcript of Christopher Porco’s Interrogation (2004). TimesUnion.com. Retrieved October 22,
2010 from http://web.timesunion.com/specialreports/porco/graphics/PORCO111504

Weisman, A. M., & Sharma, K. K., (1997). Forensic analysis and psycholegal implications of
parricide and attempted parricide. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 42(6), 1107–1113.

Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides as a
function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 11(3), 436–445.

Copyright of Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling is the property of John Wiley & Sons,

Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright

holder’s express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Place your order
(550 words)

Approximate price: $22

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more
Live Chat+1 763 309 4299EmailWhatsApp

Order your essay today and save 15% with the discount code ESSAYHELP